البوغبیش، س.؛ عابدان زاده، ر.؛ شتاب بوشهری، ن وافخم، دانشفر. (در درست چاپ). «بررسی اثر استروپ بر دوره بیپاسخی روانشناختی سالمندان». مجله سالمندی ایران.
رستمی، ر و جلیلوند، م (1395). «تأثیر تمرین بر تناوب تکلیف دوگانه دیداری شنیداری و شنیداری دیداری در دوره بیپاسخی روانشناختی». عصب روانشناسی. سال 2، دوره 2، صفحه.
Armstrong, C.L. & Cloud, B. (1998). "The emergence of spatial rotation deficits in dementia and normal aging". Neuropsychology, 12(2), 208.
Baddeley, A. (2010). "Working memory". Current biology, 20(4), R136-R140.
Craik, K. (1948). "Theory of the human operator in control systems". British Journal of Psychology. General Section, 38(3), 142-148.
Dunbar, K. & MacLeod, C.M. (1984). "A horse race of a different color: Stroop interference patterns with transformed words". Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 10(5), 622.
Dyer, F.N. (1971). "The duration of word meaning responses: Stroop interference for different preexposures of the word". Psychonomic Science, 25(4), 229-231.
Fagot, C. & Pashler, H. (1992). "Making two responses to a single object: Implications for the central attentional bottleneck". Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 18(4), 1058.
Fischer, R. & Plessow, F. (2015). "Efficient multitasking: parallel versus serial processing of multiple tasks". Frontiers in Psychology. Review. 6, 1366, 1-11.
Hongwei, G., Koceja, D., Surburg, P. & Morris, H. (2005). "Fractionated Reaction Time Using the Psychological Refractory Period Paradigm". Doctoral Dissertation, Indiana University, Bloomington.
Janczyk, M., Augst, S. & Kunde, W. (2014). "The locus of the emotional Stroop effect: A study with the PRP paradigm". Acta psychologica, 151, 8-15.
Kapoula, Z., Lê, T.T., Bonnet, A., Bourtoire, P., Demule, E., Fauvel, C. & Yang, Q. (2010). "Poor Stroop performances in 15-year-old dyslexic teenagers". Experimental brain research, 203(2), 419-425.
Logan, G.D. & Gordon, R.D. (2001). "Executive control of visual attention in dual-task situations". Psychological review, 108(2), 393.
Logan, G. D. & Schulkind, M. D. (2000). "Parallel memory retrieval in dual-task situations: I. Semantic memory". Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 26(3), 1072.
MacLeod, C. M. (1991). "Half a century of research on the Stroop effect: an integrative review". Psychological bulletin, 109(2), 163.
Magen, H. & Cohen, A. (2002). "Action-based and vision-based selection of input: Two sources of control". Psychological Research, 66(4), 247-259.
Raha, R. & Mohammad, J. (2016). "The Effect of Exercise on Visual-Auditory and Auditory-Visual Dual Frequency Assignment in Psychological Refectory Period".2(2)5.
Schmidt, R. & Lee, T. (2015). Motor control and learning. Human kinetics.
Sternberg, R. & Sternberg, K. (2016). Cognitive psychology. Nelson Education.
Strobach, T., Schütz, A. & Schubert, T. (2015). "On the importance of Task 1 and error performance measures in PRP dual-task studies". Frontiers in psychology, 6, 403, 1-8.
Thomas, J.K. (1977). "Stroop interference with word or hue pre-exposure".Doctoral dissertation, ProQuest Information & Learning.
Van Maanen, L. van Rijn, H. & Borst, J.P. (2009). "Stroop and picture-word interference are two sides of the same coin". Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 16(6), 987-999.
Welford, A.T. (1952). "The ‘psychological refractory period’and the timing of high-speed performance-a review and a theory". British Journal of Psychology. General Section, 43(1), 2-19.
Zylberberg, A. Ouellette, B. Sigman, M. & Roelfsema, P.R. (2012). "Decision making during the psychological refractory period". Current Biology, 22(19), 1795-1799.